Eye and Visual Perception
Structure of the Eye
The sensory organ for the perception of optical stimuli is the eye. The human eyeball (bulb) is spherical and has a diameter of approximately . Made of a transparent, gelatinous substance, the vitreous body (corpus vitreum), which fills the entire interior, is surrounded and held in shape by three layers of tissue (Figure 1).
The outermost eye tissue, the sclera, has little elasticity and is responsible for shaping it. In the anterior part of the eye, the sclera merges with the transparent cornea in front of the lens.
The middle layer is composed of three sections. Three quarters of the eyeball is enclosed by the choroid, which is interspersed by numerous blood vessels. In front, around the lens, the choroid merges into the corpus ciliare, which contains the ciliary muscle, which provides accommodation (focusing) by changing the curvature of the lens. The aqueous humor is also formed in the corpus ciliare. The ratio of production and reabsorption of the aqueous humor determines the intraocular pressure. Even further in front is the iris, which determines the amount of light entering the eye by altering the pupil (“iris diaphragm” in photography).
The innermost eye tissue resting on the vitreous body is the retina, which in turn consists of several layers.
The Retina
The retina is the carrier of photoreceptors as well as several layers of nerves that perform initial processing of visual stimuli. It enables the perception of intensity, wavelength and spatial allocation of incident optical stimuli.
The detection of optical stimuli begins in the layer of the retina facing away from the light, which carries the photoreceptors (“inverse” eye). The approximately rods enable black and white (light/dark) vision, while three-wavelength cones serve the color perception (Figure 2).
The spectral range covered by the eye extends from (blue) to (red), with a maximum overall sensitivity at (green). The maxima of spectral sensitivities for the three types of cones are (yellow-green), (blue-green), and (violet). Simplified, we often speak of “blue,” “green,” and “red” cones. Of all the cones found in the retina, about are red, are green, but only are blue ([1], Figure 3). The rods reach their spectral maximum at and make no contribution to color perception.
Intensity Range and Adaptation
The intensity over which the eye can process stimuli spans a range of (Table 1) and is managed by different mechanisms of adaptation:
- Use of two differently sensitive receptor types (rods and cones).
- Change of the amount of incident light through the pupil (ratio – fast process[1])
- Buildup and breakdown of visual pigment in the receptors (slow).
- Adaptive spatial and temporal stimulus integration in the retina.
The sensitivity of rods is about times higher than that of cones. At low luminances (twilight), therefore, the cones make no contribution, with the result that color perception is possible only with sufficient illumination.
Illuminance [lx] | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rods | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | |||||
Cones | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | yes | |||
Scope | scotopic | mesopic | photopic |
Table 1: Absolute sensitivity of rods and cones [2].
If the rods[2] are missing due to a damage of the eye, only day vision (mesopic and photopic vision) is possible and night blindness occurs (no scotopic vision). The eye, with an absolute intensity threshold of , is more sensitive by a factor of compared to the ear.
Spatial Resolution
In an area of about degrees around that point where the optical axis of the eye meets the retina, within the yellow spot (macula; lat. macula) lies the visual pit (fovea centralis; fovea: lat. pit). In this zone of sharpest vision there are almost only cones with a density of (for comparison: a standard TV (Low Definition/PAL) picture has pixels). Here, the nerve tracts that otherwise overlie the receptors are shifted toward the edge of the macula, forming the macular wall, allowing light to fall directly on the cones. The macula therefore exhibits high spatial resolution and good color recognition. Because of the absence of rods, scotopic vision is not possible in the foveal region (the fovea is “night blind” so to speak), but because the cones are not covered by other cells, photopic sensitivity is greater within the fovea than elsewhere in the retina.
A distance of on the retina covers a visual field of . Within the fovea centralis, objects can be discriminated at a distance of to arcminutes, corresponding to a distance on the retina of to . Outside the fovea centralis, the resolution is only around (corresponding to a distance of ).
Accommodation (Focusing)
The focusing of the eye (accommodation) is done by changing the refractive power of the lens. The far accommodated eye (setting at infinity) has a refractive power of about diopters[3] ( focal length). At maximum near accommodation (setting to ), the refractive power increases by about diopters[4]. The exact values for the so-called emmetropic (normally sighted) eye are given in the literature as follows (Table 2):
State | Location | Refractive power [dpt] | Focal length [mm] |
---|---|---|---|
Accommodative rest | Cornea and anterior chamber | ||
Lens and vitreous body | |||
Total (equivalent system) | |||
Max. Accommodation | Total (equivalent system) |
Table 2: Refractive power of the emmetropic eye [3].
Thus, approximately of the total refractive power is applied by the cornea and anterior chamber and by the lens and vitreous body. Contraction of the ciliary muscle relaxes the zonular fibers. The lens becomes thicker due to its elasticity, the refractive power is increased, and the eye accommodates to near (Figure 4).
Blink
The eyelid serves to protect and moisten the eyeball. The blink (closing of the upper eyelid) is either involuntary-periodic, voluntary or reflexive. The frequency with which the involuntary blink occurs is highly dependent on the individual. An average guideline is to blinks/min [4]. The eyelid blink frequency increases during long-lasting activities with high visual demand and can then reach values up to more than eyelid blinks/min [3].
The duration of a blink (time during which the eye remains closed) is reported in the literature to be less than for involuntary blinks, but greater than for voluntary blinks [5].
Preprocessing and Information Aggregation in the Retina
In addition to the receptor cells (on the right in Figure 2), there are four other layers of neurons in the retina that already provide “wiring” of signals within the retina and thus make a significant contribution to image preprocessing. The efficiency of these layers is evident from the fact alone that million receptors (sum of about million rods and million cones) face only million nerve fibers (ganglion cell axons) in the optic nerve. In other words, we register a convergence of between receptors and optic nerve, without, however, information recorded by the eye being simply lost at this step.
The Receptive Fields
Studies of the relationships between optical stimuli and neural activity deployed by the retina yielded rather puzzling and inexplicable results until the middle of the century. The eye exposed to complete darkness showed moderate neural activity (“spontaneous firing”). But contrary to expectations, this did not increase significantly when exposed to diffuse light. Figure 5 schematically shows the experimental setup in which a receptive field is diffusely illuminated. The action potential is derived at the ganglion cell (optic nerve) axon.
It was not until that Stephen Kuffler was able to show that retinal ganglion cells do not respond to diffuse illumination per se, but only to local illumination contrasts. A punctate exposure at or near that location (= center of the receptive field) from which the derivation was made leads to a marked increase in the impulse rate (Figure 6 – left). Exposure of a ring-shaped zone (= surrounding of the receptive field) around the derivation site has an inhibitory effect, so that even spontaneous firing disappears (Figure 6 – right).
More detailed investigations showed that there are two types of ganglion cells which differ according to the way they respond to exposure of their receptive field. In both cases, a distinction must be made between the circular center of the respective receptive field and an antagonistic ring-shaped surrounding. Through this principle, known as lateral inhibition, an increase in image sharpness is achieved.
Measurement of Visual Acuity
The visual performance of the eye is indicated by the visual acuity, which more precisely is the resolving power of the eye. Visual acuity (V) is the reciprocal of the smallest angle (Wmin in minutes of arc) at which two points can still be perceived separately.
Under good lighting conditions, a normally sighted eye can just keep two points apart when they appear at an angle of arcminute ().
The visual acuity is then .
In principle, all objects for which a significant detail appears at the angle of from the respective specified distance (optotypes) are suitable for determining visual acuity. For this purpose, e.g., visual test charts with letters (Snellen chart, Figure 7) or rings (Landolt rings, Figure 8) can be used. The slit in the rings is seen from the “target distance” below . The visual power is calculated from the quotient of actual distance by target distance. For example, if the orientation of the slit of a Landolt ring for a target distance of can only be detected when approaching , the visual acuity is .
The visual acuity of a person with normal vision is . To read a newspaper, a visual acuity of at least is required.
Attribution/Citation
Would you like to cite, share or edit this book? This book is licensed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommericial-ShareAlike 4.0.
Third party intellectual property is appropriately marked or cited and is not part of this book. All attribution and citation information applies only to the contents of this book, which are not derived from third party sources.
Attribution
If you wish to publish or distribute this book in print, edited or unedited, the following attribution must be included on each physical page:
If you wish to publish or distribute this book edited or unedited in digital form, the following attribution must be included on each digital page:
Citation
Use the following information to cite this book.
- Author: Dr. Wolfgang L. Zagler
- Title: Rehabilitationstechnik
- Date: March 1, 2008
- Location: Vienna, Austria
- Book URL:
- Chapter URL:
Footnotes
this value refers to the juvenile eye; for conditions in old age, see later. ↩︎
Since the peripheral area of the retina is almost exclusively occupied by rods, a failure of this receptor type leads not only to night blindness but also to a loss of the peripheral visual field, resulting in so-called tunnel vision. ↩︎
diopter = reciprocal of (anterior) focal length in meters; abbreviation dpt. ↩︎
Applies to the juvenile eye and is strongly dependent on age. ↩︎
List of Abbreviations
- PAL
- Phase Alternating Line
List of Figures
Figure 2: Section through the retina – light incidence from the left; direction of conduction from right to left [7].
Figure 3: Spectral behavior of the cones: Absorption in of the total amount of incident light [8].
Figure 5: Uniform (diffuse) exposure of a receptive field [8].
Figure 6: Selective exposure of (a) center and (b) surroundings of a receptive field [8].
List of Tables
List of Sources
[1]: Colour, Contrast & Perception - Design Guidance for Internal Built Environments (Keith Bright, Geoff Cook, John Harris, ISBN: 9780704912083)
[2]: Menschliche Visuelle Wahrnehmung und ihre maschinelle Substitution für sehbehindderte Menschen (Angela Zagler)
[3]: Bestimmen von Sehhilfen (Dieter Methling, ISBN: 9783432999128)
[4]: Practical ophthalmology (Fred M. Wilson Judith E. Gurland (ed.), ISBN: 9781560550341)
[5]: Computerized Control and Communication System for Handicapped (Vidas Lauruska, ISBN: 3486237977, DOI: 10.5555/247771.247842)
[6]: SMART - Servier Medical Art (Servier Medical Art, Accessed: 2022-04-06 - CC-BY 3.0 Unported License)
[7]: Biologie des Menschen (Klaus D Mörike, Eberhard Betz, Walter Mergenthaler, ISBN: 9783494002682)
[8]: Einführung in die Psychologie - Informationsaufnahme und -verarbeitung beim Menschen (Human Information Processing) (Peter H. Lindsay, Donald A. Norman)
[9]: Auge und Gehirn (David H Hubel, ISBN: 9783922508922)